Stimați colegi,

Vă invităm să participați la Cel de-al XXIV-lea Congres SNPCAR şi a 46-a Conferinţă Naţională de Neurologie-Psihiatrie a Copilului şi Adolescentului şi Profesiuni Asociate din România cu participare internaţională

25-28 septembrie 2024 – CRAIOVA, Hotel Ramada

Vă așteptăm cu drag!

Asist. Univ. Dr. Cojocaru Adriana – Președinte SNPCAR

Informații şi înregistrări: vezi primul anunț 


THE DEVELOPMENT OF ARITHMETIC SKILLS IN CHILDREN PART II

Autor: Elena Cecilia Rosca Mihaela Simu Ruxandra Dana Chirileanu
Distribuie pe:

ABSTRACT: 

Functional neuroimaging has made remarkable progress in the last years and provided new data on numerical and calculation processes. While there is a large amount of research regarding the arithmetic skills in adults, there are currently only a few functional studies approaching the arithmetic abilities in children. Although the neuroimaging studies regarding dyscalculia have provided variable results, some consistent fi ndings have emerged so far. It is clear now that in developmental dyscalculia there is an abnormal structure and function of parietal regions. Nevertheless, further research is needed regarding the interrelation between the different aspects of arithmetic cognition as well as the breakdown of these abilities.

 


 

NEUROIMAGING ASPECTS IN CHILDREN WITH DYSCALCULIA

In the last 20 years, the development of neuroimaging technologies has led to high progresses in the cognitive neurosciences domain, methods such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), event-related brain potentials (ERP) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) enabling insights into the brain regions that are implicated in a cognitive process. Regarding the arithmetic cognition, functional neuroimaging has provided new data on numerical and calculation processes, ranging from magnitude processing to calculation. Corroborated with the evidence from adult braindamaged patients, these data provided a basis for new neurofunctional models of arithmetic cognition.

The most influential theory is the “triple code” model proposed by Dehaene and Cohen (Dehaene & Cohen, 1995; Dehaene & Cohen, 1997; Dehaene et al., 2003). Th e authors suggest that numbers are represented in the human brain in 3 distinct formats: as a sequence of words in the verbal code, as a sequence of Arabic numerals in the visual Arabic code and as analogical representations of number magnitude in the magnitude code. These 3 representations are linked by transcoding routes of various involvements, often in combination in calculation procedures. The verbal code is necessary for the retrieval of arithmetical facts (e.g. simple additions and multiplications), the visual Arabic code supports multidigit calculations and parity judgments and the magnitude code subserves semantic knowledge of numerical quantities being used in number comparisons and calculations. The verbal code has as its anatomical substrate a large cortico- striato-thalamo-cortical network comprising left perisylvian areas, the left angular gyrus and the left basal ganglia. The visual Arabic code is subserved by the temporo-occipital junction in both hemispheres. The magnitude representation is subserved by bilateral cortical areas around the intraparietal sulcus and both posterior superior parietal lobes, which are involved in the spatial and attentional processes relevant for magnitude representations of numbers along a so-called mental number line (for a review see Rosca, 2009a).

Different substrates are proposed for the arithmetic operations. The simple multiplications are considered rote learnt facts, being solved through a direct route by retrieval from memory via the verbal route. At the opposite pole are the subtractions that are solved through magnitude manipulations via an indirect semantic route. The additions are considered to be based both on rote memory and magnitude representations: the simple additions are hypothesized to be rote learnt facts, but the more complex additions are solved by magnitude manipulations. The data regarding the divisions is somewhat contradictory; some authors demonstrated that in order to solve a division it is necessary to access the corresponding rote learnt multiplication (Campbell, 1997) but other researchers did not observed signifi cant improvement in solving divisions after training of multiplication tables (Rickard et al., 1994). However, these fi ndings could be due to individual diff erences in arithmetic (Domahs & Delazer, 2005).

It is generally accepted now that in order to solve a complex arithmetic problem it is necessary to process the numerical information (to perceive, comprehend and produce numbers), to process the operational sign that indicates the specifi c calculation to be performed, to access the arithmetic facts (e. g. 4 x 8 = 32, 4 + 8 = 12), to execute the calculation procedures that specify the sequence of steps to be carried out in solving multi-digit operations (procedural knowledge) and to understand the arithmetic operations and principles (conceptual knowledge) (Sandrini et al., 2003). Recently, it has been proposed that the anatomical substrate for arithmetical procedural knowledge might be a left fronto-parieto-subcortical loop, in which the fronto-parietal and fronto-subcortical loops provide support for the monitoring and sequencing of the necessary steps of a complex calculation, and the parieto- subcortical loop supports the visuo-spatial working memory necessary for the representation of each sub-step of the procedure (Rosca, 2009b).

While there is a large amount of data regarding the numerical and arithmetic skills in adults, there are currently only a few functional studies approaching the arithmetic abilities in children, due to age limitations of some techniques. For example, fMRI is restricted to children of 4 – 5 years old (because the participants need to be awake and to respond to stimuli presented in a very noisy medium from the scanner). Temple and Posner made one of the fi rst functional studies investigating the neural basis of number processing in children. They used ERP to compare the activation of cortical areas during magnitude classifi cation in 5 – year – old children and adults and demonstrated that both, children and adults recruit parietal brain regions upon making symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude judgments (Temple & Posner, 1998). Furthermore, Isaacs et al. found reduced white matter densities in children with dyscalculia when comparing prematurely born adolescents with and without dyscalculia (Isaacs et al., 2001). In 2006, Cantlon et al. demonstrated by fMRI techniques that intraparietal sulcus is activated during nonsymbolic magnitude processing in both 4-year-old children and adults (Cantlon et al., 2006) but other researchers found that children seem to rely more on prefrontal areas upon making magnitude classifi cations compared with adults, the parietal activations in 9 – 12-year-old children being less strong and less consistent (Kaufmann et al., 2006). Recently, functional neuroimaging studies demonstrated an aberrant activation of intraparietal sulcus in dyscalculic children upon solving symbolic number tasks such as comparison of Arabic numbers (Kaufmann et al., 2009a; Mussolin et al., 2010). Th e data on non-symbolic number processing is controversial: despite the key role of the intraparietal sulcus in the magnitude processing, some studies reported comparable parietal activations in children with and without dyscalculia, without group diff erences in the non-symbolic number comparison tasks (Kucian et al., 2006). However, other researcher found a defi cient recruitment of the right intraparietal sulcus in developmental dyscalculia (Price et al., 2007). A more recent fMRI study comparing non-symbolic number magnitude processing in 9-year-old children with and without dyscalculia was suggestive for a less consistent neural activity in right intraparietal regions and compensatory neural activity in left intraparietal regions in developmental dyscalculia (Kaufmann et al., 2009b). Th e authors interpreted the signifi cantly stronger activation of left intraparietal areas as refl ecting compensatory mechanisms, children with functional defi cits needing to recruit a wider network of regions to perform the task; furthermore, the activations are stronger in order to compensate the processing difficulties.

In the adult literature it was demonstrated that focal brain injury determines specifi c impairments in number processing and calculation; based on this data, researchers proposed different cognitive models for arithmetic skills. A similar approach was also present in the developmental literature, the most widely accepted hypothesis proposing a defi cient numerosity concept as the core defi cit for dyscalculia (Butterworth, 2005). However, in developmental disorders it is more complicated to disentangle the defi cits and to conclude if there is a causal relation between the concomitant impairments, children with dyscalculia rarely exhibiting isolated cognitive defi cits in the arithmetical domain. Furthermore, developmental dyscalculia has been demonstrated to be a heterogeneous disorder, with diff erent performance profi les both between individuals and within individuals (Dowker 2005). An interesting study that compared the arithmetic calculations networks between children and adults was done by Kawashima and his colleagues. Using fMRI, they compared the neural substrates of addition, subtraction and multiplication in 9-14 year – old children and 40-49 years adults and demonstrated a broadly similar pattern of activation, with the involvement of the prefrontal, intraparietal, occipitotemporal and occipital cortex. However, there were also some subtle differences, children presenting a largely left lateralized activation of the prefrontal cortex while adults exhibited more bilateral activation of prefrontal areas (Kawashima et al, 2004). Rivera et al., investigating how brain activation underlying arithmetic calculations (additions and subtractions) changes between the ages of 8 and 19 years, demonstrated a decrease in activation with age in the prefrontal (dorsolateral and ventrolateral) cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex. The authors suggested that their fi ndings could be due to the fact that younger children use additional working memory and attention to achieve similar levels of arithmetic performance with the older children (Rivera et al, 2005).

One method of studying the developmental impairments of number processing and calculation consists in investigating populations with arithmetic disturbances occurring in the context of genetic developmental syndromes such as Turner syndrome, Williams syndrome and Fragile X syndrome. For example, Molko and his colleagues, comparing the functional and structural brain changes found in Turner syndrome with normal controls, demonstrated increased activation in the bilateral intraparietal sulcus as the diffi culty of calculation increased in normal subjects while the Turner syndrome subjects did not show the same pattern. Furthermore, the Turner syndrome subjects presented abnormal structural organization of the intraparietal sulcus (Molko et al, 2003). Th is pattern of decreased activation was also observed in subjects with Fragile X syndrome (Rivera et al, 2002). However, Kesler et al. reported that, compared with controls, children with Turner syndrome recruited additional neuronal resources in frontal and parietal regions during an easier, two-operand calculation task, whereas during a more difficult three-operand task they showed signifi cantly decreased activation in frontal, parietal and subcortical regions than controls. The authors concluded that the dyscalculic children must recruit additional brain regions during the relatively easy task and demonstrate a potentially inefficient response to increased task difficulty compared with controls (Kesler et al, 2006).

An abnormal structure of the intraparietal sulcus, with signifi cantly less gray matter in the left intraparietal sulcus was also reported by Isaacs and his colleagues in adolescents with deficits in calculation (Isaacs et al, 2001). Another study that compared the gray matter volume of brain tissue in subjects with and without dyscalculia revealed reduced amount of gray matter in the right parietal cortex as well as the frontal brain regions in subjects with dyscalculia (Rotzer et al, 2008).

A more recent MRI and diffusion tensor imaging study compared the macro and micro-structural impairments in 7-9 year-old children with dyscalculia to a group of typically developing children and reported robust gray matter and white matter deficits in key brain areas that have previously implicated in arithmetic cognition. Integrated analyses of brain structure using a combination of voxel-based morphometry and diffusion tensor imaging provided evidence for defi – cits in the dorsal and ventral visual stream. They found as key anatomical correlates of dyscalculia the right hemisphere temporo-parietal white matter, its microstructure and pathways associated with it, including most notably, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and the inferior longitudinal fasciculus. The authors suggested the possibility of multiple dysfunctional circuits arising from a core white matter defi cit and hypothesized that developmental dyscalculia, at its core, could be a disconnection syndrome (Rykhlevskaia et al, 2009). Thus developmental dyscalculia is associated with both atypical structure and function of the intraparietal sulcus.

An interesting finding regarding the neuroimaging in developmental dyscalculia was provided by Levy and his colleagues. They reported the study of an 18 years-old severely dyscalculic male in which, the conventional MRI scans showed no abnormalities but the magnetic resonance spectroscopy revealed a focal defect in the left temporo-parietal area, in the region of angular gyrus, including defects in metabolite amplitudes (Levy et al, 1999). This study highlighted the importance of using multiple methods to capture a particular cognitive function.

Despite the fact that the neuroimaging studies regarding dyscalculia have provided variable results, some consistent findings have emerged so far. It is clear now that in developmental dyscalculia there is an abnormal structure and function of parietal regions, but its role in arithmetical cognition is not completely elucidated.

In children, functional neuroimaging contributes to clinical diagnosis of certain neurological diseases and ads to our understanding of the development of arithmetic skills, although there are many conceptual and methodological difficulties. So far, these studies demonstrated that the circuits implicated in mathematical cognition are both structurally and functionally impaired in developmental dyscalculia. Nevertheless, further research is needed regarding the interrelation between the different aspects of arithmetic cognition as well as the breakdown of these abilities, in order to diagnose and remediate children with developmental dyscalculia and to conduct an adequate teaching of mathematics.

 

BIBLIOGRAFIE / BIBLIOGRAPHY

1. Butterworth B. Th e development of arithmetic abilities. Journal of Child Psichology and Psychiatry 2005; 46: 3-18.

2. Campbell JID. On the relation between skilled performance of simple division and multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 1997; 23: 1140-1159.

3. Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4- year-old children. PLOS Biology 2006; 4(5): 844-854.

4. Dehaene S, Cohen L. Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition 1995; 1: 83-120.

5. Dehaene S, Cohen L. Cerebral pathways for calculation: Double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex 1997; 33: 219-250.

6. Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive neuropsychology 2003; 20: 487-506.

7. Domahs F, Delazer M. Some assumptions and facts about arithmetic facts. Psychology Science 2005; 47(1): 96-111.

8. Dowker A. Individual diff erences in arithmetic. Psychology Press, 2005.

9. Isaacs EB, Edmonds CJ, Lucas A, Gadian DG. Calculation diffi culties in children of very low birth weight. Brain 2001; 124(9): 1701-1707.

10. Kaufmann L, Koppelstaetter F, Siedentopf C, Haala I, Haberlandt E, Zimmerhackl LB, Felber S, Ischebeck A. Neural correlates of a number-size interference task in children. NeuroReport 2006; 17(6): 587-591.

11. Kaufmann L, Vogel SE, Starke M, Kremser C, Schocke M. Numerical and non-numerical ordinality processing in children with and without developmental dyscalculia: Evidence from fMRI. Cognitive Development 2009; 24(4): 486-494 (a).

12. Kaufmann L, Vogel SE, Starke M, Kremser C, Schocke M, Wood G. Developmental dyscalculia: compensatory mechanisms in left intraparietal regions in response to nonsymbolic magnitudes. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2009; 5: 35 (b).

13. Kawashima R, Taira M, Okita K, Inoue K, Tajima N, Yoshida H, Sasaki T, Sugiura M, Watanabe J, Fukuda H. A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic – A comparison between children and adults. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 2004; 18(3): 227-233.

14. Kesler SR, Menon V, Reiss AL. Neuro-functional differences associated with arithmetic processing in Turner syndrome. Cerebral Cortex 2006; 16: 849-856.

15. Kucian K, Loenneker T, Dietrich T, Dosch M, Martin E, Von Aster M. Impaired neuronal networks for approximate calculation in dyscalculic children: A functional MRI study. Behavioural and Brain Functions 2006; 2: 31.

16. Levy LM, Reiss IL, Grafman J. Metabolic abnormalities detected by 1 H – MRS in dyscalculia and dysgraphia. Neurology 1999; 53(3): 639-641.

17. Molko N, Cachia A, Riviere D, Mangin JF, Bruandet M, Le Bihan D, Cohen L, Dehaene S. Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron 2003; 40(4): 847-858.

18. Mussolin C, De Volder A, Grandin C, Schlogel X, Nassogne MC, Noel MP. Neural correlates of symbolic number comparison in developmental dyscalculia. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences 2010; 22(5): 860-874.

19. Price GR, Holloway I, Rasanen P, Vesterinen M, Ansari D. Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia. Current Biology 2007; 17: R1042.

20. Rickard TC, Healy AF, Bourne LE. On the cognitive structure of basic arithmetic skills: operation, order and symbol transfer eff ects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 1994; 20: 1139-1153.

21. Rivera SM, Menon V, White CD, Glaser B, Reiss AL. Functional brain activation during arithmetic processing in females with fragile X syndrome is related to FMR1 protein expression. Human Brain Mapping 2002; 16(4): 206-218.

22. Rivera SM, Reiss AL, Eckert MA, Menon V. Developmental changes in mental arithmetic: evidence for increased specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 2005; 15: 1779-1790.

23. Rosca EC. Aritmetica – o perspectivă neuropsihologica. Artpress, Timisoara 2009 (a)

24. Rosca EC. Arithmetic procedural knowledge: a corticosubcortical circuit. Brain Research 2009; 1302: 148-156 (b).

25. Rotzer S, Kucian K, Martin E, Aster MV, Klaver P, Loenneker, T. Optimized voxel-based morphometry in children with developmental dyscalculia. Neuroimage, 2008; 39(1): 417-422.

26. Rykhlevskaia E, Uddin LQ, Kondos L, Menon V. Neuroanatomical correlates of developmental dyscalculia: combined evidence from morphometry and tractography. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2009, 3: 51.

27. Sandrini M, Miozzo A, Cotelli M, Cappa S. The residual calculation abilities of a patient with severe aphasia: evidence for a selective defi cit of subtraction procedures. Cortex 2003; 39: 85-96.

28. Temple E, Posner M. Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-yaer-olds and adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 1998; 95: 7836-7841.